A. Nobre, Probing the flexibility of attentional orienting in the human brain Cognitive neuroscience of attention, pp.157-79, 2004.

A. Nobre, G. Rohenkohl, and M. Stokes, Nervous Anticipation: Top-down biasing across Space and Time, Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, pp.159-86, 2012.

S. Petersen and M. Posner, The Attention System of the Human Brain: 20 Years After, Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol.35, issue.1, pp.73-89, 2012.
DOI : 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525

J. Coull, C. Frith, C. Buchel, and A. Nobre, Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts, Neuropsychologia, vol.38, issue.6, pp.808-827, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00132-3

A. Vallesi and T. Shallice, Developmental dissociations of preparation over time: Deconstructing the variable foreperiod phenomena., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.33, issue.6, pp.1377-88, 2007.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1377

D. Muir, D. Humphrey, and G. Humphrey, Pattern and space perception in young infants, Spatial Vision, vol.8, issue.1, pp.141-65, 1994.
DOI : 10.1163/156856894X00288

S. Robertson, S. Watamura, and M. Wilbourn, Attentional dynamics of infant visual foraging, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol.109, issue.28, p.22733754, 2012.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.1203482109

M. Corbetta and G. Shulman, CONTROL OF GOAL-DIRECTED AND STIMULUS-DRIVEN ATTENTION IN THE BRAIN, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol.3, issue.3, pp.201-216, 2002.
DOI : 10.1038/nrn755

C. Harmon, M. Posner, M. Rothbart, and L. Thomas-thrapp, Development of orienting to locations and objects in human infants., Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie exp??rimentale, vol.48, issue.2, pp.301-319, 1994.
DOI : 10.1037/1196-1961.48.2.301

J. Richards, Localizing the development of covert attention in infant using scalp event-related-potentials, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.21, issue.1, pp.91-108, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(98)91859-8

J. Fan, B. Mccandliss, T. Sommer, A. Raz, and M. Posner, Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol.41, issue.3, pp.340-347, 2002.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.080070897

M. Posner, Orienting of attention, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.197, issue.1, pp.3-25, 1980.
DOI : 10.1080/00335558008248231

K. Johnson, I. Robertson, E. Barry, A. Mulligan, A. Dáibhis et al., Impaired conflict resolution and alerting in children with ADHD: evidence from the Attention Network Task (ANT), Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, vol.47, issue.Suppl., pp.1339-1386, 2008.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01936.x

M. Rueda, J. Fan, B. Mccandliss, J. Halparin, D. Gruber et al., Development of attentional networks in childhood, Neuropsychologia, vol.42, issue.8, p.15093142, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012

L. Trainor, Predictive information processing is a fundamental learning mechanism present in early development: Evidence from infants, International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol.83, issue.2, pp.256-264, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.12.008

S. Trehub and L. Thorpe, Infants' perception of rhythm: Categorization of auditory sequences by temporal structure., Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, vol.43, issue.2, pp.217-246, 1989.
DOI : 10.1037/h0084223

J. Colombo and W. Richman, Infant Timekeeping: Attention and Temporal Estimation in 4-Month-Olds, Psychological Science, vol.13, issue.5, pp.475-484, 2002.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-9280.00484

E. Brannon, S. Suanda, and K. Libertus, Temporal discrimination increases in precision over development and parallels the development of numerosity discrimination, Developmental Science, vol.74, issue.1, pp.770-777, 2007.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00508.x

K. Vanmarle and K. Wynn, Six-month-old infants use analog magnitudes to represent duration, Developmental Science, vol.48, issue.2, pp.41-50, 2006.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00395.x

J. Provasi, A. Rattat, and S. Droit-volet, Temporal bisection in 4-month-old infants., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, vol.37, issue.1, pp.108-121, 2011.
DOI : 10.1037/a0019976

S. Droit-volet, A. Clément, and J. Wearden, Temporal Generalization in 3- to 8-Year-Old Children, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.80, issue.3, pp.271-88, 2001.
DOI : 10.1006/jecp.2001.2629

S. Droit-volet and J. Wearden, Temporal Bisection in Children, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.80, issue.2, pp.142-59, 2001.
DOI : 10.1006/jecp.2001.2631

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00158751

D. Berwanger, M. Wittmann, V. Steinbüchel, N. , V. Suchodoletz et al., Measurement of temporal-order judgment in children, Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars), vol.64, issue.3, pp.387-94, 2004.

S. Droit-volet and P. Zélanti, Development of Time Sensitivity and Information Processing Speed, PLoS ONE, vol.14, issue.10, p.23990953, 2013.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0071424.t006

P. Zélanti and S. Droit-volet, Cognitive abilities explaining age-related changes in time perception of short and long durations, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.109, issue.2, pp.143-57, 2011.
DOI : 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.01.003

S. Droit-volet, Time perception in children: A neurodevelopmental approach, Neuropsychologia, vol.51, issue.2, pp.220-254, 2013.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.023

J. Provasi, D. Anderson, and M. Barbu-roth, Rhythm perception, production, and synchronization during the perinatal period, Frontiers in Psychology, vol.32, issue.1430, pp.1-16, 1048.
DOI : 10.1038/jp.2011.177

M. Besson, J. Chobert, and C. Marie, Transfer of Training between Music and Speech: Common Processing, Attention, and Memory, Frontiers in Psychology, vol.2, issue.94, pp.1-23, 2011.
DOI : 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00094

A. Khalil, V. Minces, G. Mcloughlin, and A. Chiba, Group rhythmic synchrony and attention in children, Frontiers in Psychology, vol.4, p.24032021, 2013.
DOI : 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00564

N. Cason and D. Schön, Rhythmic priming enhances the phonological processing of speech, Neuropsychologia, vol.50, issue.11, pp.2652-2660, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.018

E. Flaugnacco, L. Lopez, C. Terribili, S. Zoia, S. Buda et al., Rhythm perception and production predict reading abilities in developmental dyslexia, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol.4, issue.25, pp.1-14, 2014.
DOI : 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00025

J. Coull and A. Nobre, Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI, J Neurosci, vol.18, issue.18, pp.7426-7461, 1998.

I. Griffin, C. Miniussi, and A. Nobre, Multiple mechanisms of selective attention: differential modulation of stimulus processing by attention to space or time, Neuropsychologia, vol.40, issue.13, pp.2325-2365, 2002.
DOI : 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00087-8

J. Coull, A. Nobre, and C. Frith, The Noradrenergic ??2 Agonist Clonidine Modulates Behavioural and Neuroanatomical Correlates of Human Attentional Orienting and Alerting, Cerebral Cortex, vol.11, issue.1, pp.73-84, 2001.
DOI : 10.1093/cercor/11.1.73

J. Coull, H. Morgan, V. Cambridge, J. Moore, F. Giorlando et al., Ketamine perturbs perception of the flow of time in healthy volunteers, Psychopharmacology, vol.133, issue.1074, pp.543-56, 2011.
DOI : 10.1007/s00213-011-2346-9

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01384142

R. Luce, . Response-times, D. Broadbent, J. Mcgaugh, N. Mackintosh et al., Their role in inferring elementary mental organization, 1986.

H. Woodrow, The measurement of attention., The Psychological Monographs, vol.17, issue.5, pp.1-158, 1914.
DOI : 10.1037/h0093087

P. Niemi and R. Näätänen, Foreperiod and simple reaction time., Psychological Bulletin, vol.89, issue.1, pp.133-62, 1981.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-2909.89.1.133

R. Elliot, Simple reaction time: Effects associated with age, preparatory interval, incentive-shift, and mode of presentation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.9, issue.1, pp.86-107, 1970.
DOI : 10.1016/0022-0965(70)90102-5

A. Baumeister and C. Joubert, Interactive effects on reaction time of preparatory interval length and preparatory interval frequency., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.82, issue.2, pp.393-398, 1969.
DOI : 10.1037/h0028119

L. Karlin, Reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration and variability., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.58, issue.2, pp.185-91, 1959.
DOI : 10.1037/h0049152

S. Los and C. Van-den-heuvel, Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.27, issue.2, pp.370-86, 2001.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.370

A. Vallesi, S. Arbula, and P. Bernardis, Functional dissociations in temporal preparation: Evidence from dual-task performance, Cognition, vol.130, issue.2, pp.141-51, 2014.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.10.006

A. Vallesi, V. Lozano, and A. Correa, Dissociating temporal preparation processes as a function of the inter-trial interval duration, Cognition, vol.127, issue.1, pp.22-30, 2013.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.011

P. Greenwood and R. Parasuraman, Attentional disengagement deficit in nondemented elderly over 75 years of age, Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, vol.46, issue.3, pp.188-202, 1994.
DOI : 10.1080/00223980.1945.9917223

T. Zanto, P. Pan, H. Liu, J. Bollinger, A. Nobre et al., Age-Related Changes in Orienting Attention in Time, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.31, issue.35, pp.12461-70, 2011.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1149-11.2011

G. Rohenkohl, J. Coull, and A. Nobre, Behavioural Dissociation between Exogenous and Endogenous Temporal Orienting of Attention, PLoS ONE, vol.6, issue.1, p.21297968, 2011.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0014620.g002

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01384786

A. Breska and L. Deouell, Automatic Bias of Temporal Expectations following Temporally Regular Input Independently of High-level Temporal Expectation, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol.26, issue.7, pp.1555-71, 2014.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.121

S. Los and D. Heslenfeld, Intentional and Unintentional Contributions to Nonspecific Preparation: Electrophysiological Evidence., Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, vol.134, issue.1, pp.52-72, 2005.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-3445.134.1.52

G. Mento, V. Tarantino, A. Vallesi, and P. Bisiacchi, Spatiotemporal Neurodynamics Underlying Internally and Externally Driven Temporal Prediction: A High Spatial Resolution ERP Study, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol.27, issue.3
DOI : 10.1037/h0093087

A. Vallesi, A. Mcintosh, T. Shallice, and D. Stuss, When Time Shapes Behavior: fMRI Evidence of Brain Correlates of Temporal Monitoring, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol.5, issue.6, pp.1116-1142, 2009.
DOI : 10.1901/jeab.2005.121-04

M. Triviño, M. Arnedo, J. Lupiáñez, J. Chirivella, and A. Correa, Rhythms can overcome temporal orienting deficit after right frontal damage, Neuropsychologia, vol.49, issue.14, p.22019698, 2011.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.10.009

E. Brannon, L. Roussel, W. Meck, and M. Woldorff, Timing in the baby brain, Cognitive Brain Research, vol.21, issue.2, pp.227-260, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.04.007

J. Doherty, A. Rao, M. Mesulam, and A. Nobre, Synergistic Effect of Combined Temporal and Spatial Expectations on Visual Attention, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.25, issue.36, pp.8259-66, 2005.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1821-05.2005

G. Rohenkohl, I. Gould, J. Pessoa, and A. Nobre, Combining spatial and temporal expectations to improve visual perception, Journal of Vision, vol.14, issue.4, 2014.
DOI : 10.1167/14.4.8

J. Coull, K. Davranche, B. Nazarian, and F. Vidal, Functional anatomy of timing differs for production versus prediction of time intervals, Neuropsychologia, vol.51, issue.2, pp.309-328, 2013.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.017

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00773342

J. Cotti, G. Rohenkohl, M. Stokes, A. Nobre, and J. Coull, Functionally dissociating temporal and motor components of response preparation in left intraparietal sulcus, NeuroImage, vol.54, issue.2, pp.1221-1251, 2011.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.038

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01384791

J. Coull, F. Vidal, C. Goulon, B. Nazarian, and C. Craig, Using Time-To-Contact information to assess potential collision modulates both visual and temporal prediction networks, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol.2, issue.10, pp.1-12, 2008.
DOI : 10.3389/neuro.09.010.2008

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01384857

J. Marchant and J. Driver, Visual and Audiovisual Effects of Isochronous Timing on Visual Perception and Brain Activity, Cerebral Cortex, vol.23, issue.6, pp.1290-1298, 2013.
DOI : 10.1093/cercor/bhs095

D. Bolger, J. Coull, and D. Schön, Metrical Rhythm Implicitly Orients Attention in Time as Indexed by Improved Target Detection and Left Inferior Parietal Activation, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol.20, issue.3, pp.593-605, 2014.
DOI : 10.1037/h0093087

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01384043

S. Bayless, Y. Nagata, T. Mills, and M. Taylor, MEG Measures of Covert Orienting and Gaze Processing in Children, Brain Topography, vol.82, issue.Pt 3, pp.616-642, 2013.
DOI : 10.1007/s10548-013-0279-9

B. Olk, B. Cameron, and A. Kingstone, Enhanced orienting effects: Evidence for an interaction principle, Visual Cognition, vol.35, issue.7, pp.979-1000, 2008.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.121

J. Ristic and A. Kingstone, Attention to arrows: Pointing to a new direction, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.59, issue.11, pp.1921-1951, 2006.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03196287

J. Ristic, C. Friesen, and A. Kingstone, Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, vol.8, issue.3, pp.507-520, 2002.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03196306

J. Ristic and A. Kingstone, Rethinking attentional development: reflexive and volitional orienting in children and adults, Developmental Science, vol.17, issue.2, pp.289-96, 2009.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00756.x

B. Olk, Effects of spatial, temporal and spatiotemporal cueing are alike when attention is directed voluntarily, Experimental Brain Research, vol.16, issue.1, pp.3623-3656, 2014.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-014-4033-7